Raising Perceptual Phonemic Awareness in the EFL Classroom

نویسنده

  • Esther Gómez
چکیده

This study examined the impact of phonetic training on English lexical schwa vowel in primary school learners (aged 12) of English as a foreign language in Spain. Students’ perceptual awareness on the occurrence of schwa in unstressed position in English was tested in three groups: i) a group which underwent auditory discrimination and identification practice, ii) a group which underwent listen‐and‐repeat practice, iii) a control group with native exposure. The first two groups could consistently identify full vowels as incorrect in post‐ test. Few differences in perceptual awareness as a function of position of the unstressed vowel in the word (pre vs. post‐tonic) were found. Word familiarity was found to affect pre‐test performance but to be hindered after treatments. Results acknowledge the positive impact of controlled phonetic training on L2 sound perceptual awareness in classroom settings and contribute to the development of L2 pronunciation teaching and learning. Recent research in L2 speech perception and production training research has been found to favour the development of new sound categories (Bradlow, 2008 for a review on /r/‐/l/; Cenoz & Garcia Lecumberri, 1999; Hazan et al., 2006; Iverson et al., 2012; Pruitt et al. 2006; Wang, 2000; Wayland & Li, 2008). Much of this research has been motivated by a wish to investigate the nature of the acquisition process of the sounds of the Esther Gómez Lacabex and Francisco Gallardo del Puerto 204 second/foreign language system in adulthood, given that late language learners experience difficulties in perceiving and producing L2 sounds (Flege & McKay, 2011). Many of these training studies have looked into variables such as robustness (Pruitt et al., 2006) and durability (Lively et al., 1994; Wang, 2000) of training effects as well as the relevance of feedback provision (Jamieson & Morosan, 1986; Strange & Dittman, 1984). Some have also explored the effects of different training procedures such as perception‐based versus production‐based training tasks (Leather, 1996), effects of perceptual discrimination versus perceptual identification tasks (Flege, 1995) or effects of training with different acoustic manipulations in natural and varied stimuli (Iverson et al., 2005). These variables have mainly been researched in rigid laboratory settings, where training tends to be fixed, short‐termed, and with adult learners. Although these research lines may be of interest in pedagogically‐oriented research, pronunciation teaching does not seem to have benefitted from them and is often referred to still hold a marginal position in applied linguistics these days (Barrera Pardo, 2004; Derwing & Munro, 2005; Jones, 1997; Setter & Jenkins, 2005). Those studies which have tested these training procedures in more instructional settings are scarce. Wang and Munro (2004) devised some training software based on perceptual identification tasks with synthetic and natural speech in various voices for some vowel contrasts. This protocol was administered to a group of university students who worked on the training blocks at their own pace over a period of two months and could replay them, if desired. The authors stated that such a procedure can be pedagogically applied as a self‐study resource or extra computer‐based practice as it addressed individual needs. In Spain, studies like Aliaga‐García and Mora (2009) or Cenoz and García Lecumberri (1999) have also administered specific phonetic training based on controlled perceptual discrimination and identification practice as well as production practice within broader training courses which included lectures on phonetics. However, no studies have been carried out with younger foreign language learners without metaphonetic awareness. The availability of technological resources in primary and secondary education these days allows for the possibility of performing these training procedures in more pedagogically‐oriented settings and exploring their impact in new and genuine learning environments. The present study aims at contributing to research done in the field of L2 sound training outside laboratory settings by examining the impact of classroom phonetic training on English lexical schwa vowel by primary school learners of English as a foreign language. Students’ awareness on Raising Perceptual Phonemic Awareness in EFL 205 the nature and occurrence of schwa in English unstressed syllables was tested in a perceptual identification task which presented both spliced correct schwas and spliced incorrect full vowels in unstressed syllables in English content words (ago, seven). Perceptual awareness was investigated as a function of position of the unstressed syllable within the word (pre‐ tonic and post‐tonic) and as a function of word familiarity (Flege et al., 1996). METHOD Participants Seventy‐five Basque/Spanish 6th graders (aged 12) learning English as a foreign language and distributed in three intact groups of 25 students each took part in the experiment. Two of these groups were assigned two different phonetic training treatments: group A (10 boys and 15 girls) underwent a training regime based on perception tasks and group B (8 boys and 17 girls) underwent listen‐and‐repeat‐practice. The third group (C) (8 boys and 17 girls) received no phonetic training but had a native teacher for English and Arts and Crafts. All the groups had started learning English at the age of 3 in the school, which was enrolled in a language programme that integrated three languages in the school curriculum averagely: Spanish, Basque (both official in the region) and English. The subjects which were taught in English were English, Arts and Crafts, P.E. and Science. Stimuli and Testing Procedure Fifty real two‐syllable words which included a lexical schwa in pre‐tonic (alarm) and post‐tonic (salad) unstressed position were selected in a 1 to 2 distribution respecting the frequency of occurrence of each pattern in English: 34 items were chosen for post‐tonic lexical schwa and 16 items for pre‐tonic lexical schwa. The items were recorded in a sound‐proof booth by three native speakers of English with knowledge of Spanish (two females and one male). These speakers were first asked to elicit the English words twice, which were presented to them orthographically, and then they were asked to elicit the same words mimicking Spanish pronunciation by trying to colour the schwas towards Spanish vowel qualities. A native Spanish speaker highly proficient in English supervised these recordings and supported the informants, when necessary. Schwa‐ Esther Gómez Lacabex and Francisco Gallardo del Puerto 206 full vowel minimal pairs were created by splicing the mimicked Spanish full vowel and the schwa of the second elicitation and pasting them to the same baseline for each informant’s word using PRAAT. The audio input chosen for the testing sessions was one of the female voices. The audio files recorded by the other two speakers were used in the training sessions. All audio sound files were normalised to the same Root Mean Squared (RMS) level. The perceptual awareness test (47 items1) presented words with a spliced correct schwa (eg: /əgri:/) and words with a spliced incorrect full vowel (e.g: /əgri:/) in a random manner (along with a set of 8 distractors) which students had to judge as a correctly pronounced word (click on a happy smiley) or incorrectly pronounced word (click on a sad smile). The elicited word was presented orthographically for each listening trial and the vowel in the unstressed syllable was underlined (woman) as instructions explained that the potential incorrectness would always be in the underlined sound, a vowel, never in the rest of the word. Instructions were delivered using both English and Spanish to ensure understanding of the task. Prior to the pre‐test, word familiarity was controlled by means of a written test in which the students ticked whether they knew the word or not. This information was used to distribute the items averagely (same amount of known and unknown words by means of calculating median value) in each training session. The items were also averagely and proportionally distributed into training sessions and tasks according to speaker voice (male/female) and stress pattern (pre/post‐tonic). Training Four training periods were programmed to be delivered in 6 sessions so as to cater for absentees or slower learners. An introductory session reviewed the notions of syllable, stressed syllable, unstressed syllable and unstressed vowel and provided instruction for the practice sessions. In this session, the group undergoing listen‐and‐repeat practice was additionally given two pronunciation tips: i) prepare your lips for the first consonant in words which start with a vowel, as in amend and ii) imagine the underlying score moves up and crosses the vowel in between consonants, as in raisin, so the consonants are pronounced subsequently. 1 Three items had to be eliminated because cross‐splicing produced too much artifact in the voice of the speaker used in the testing. This did not happen in the voices of the two speakers used in training; it was decided not to eliminate them from training. Raising Perceptual Phonemic Awareness in EFL 207 Sessions were always supervised by an instructor and a technician, both of whom ensured that tasks were completed sensitively and that audio and microphone equipments were working. The training regime was based on perceptual practice and included discrimination and identification tasks of the type: i) same/different discrimination task (12 items), ii) oddity discrimination task (13 items), iii) correct/incorrect identification task (25 items), iv) correct/incorrect identification task with confidence rating (12 items). Feedback was delivered by flashing a happy smile and providing a cumulative star every time a trial was correct in the first attempt. If the trial was incorrect, the sequence would play again and the student had to provide a new answer. No stars were given in subsequent attempts. The training regime for group B was based on imitation practice. Students heard the word, which they also had on the screen orthographically signaled so as to reinforce vowel focus, and repeated it in a MATLAB interface, which recorded their productions. The programme forced them to listen to their own recording once and they could repeat the elicitation two more times if they were not satisfied with their productions. The supervising instructor monitored the productions throughout the sessions and provided positive feedback to those students who attempted at producing a reduced vowel successfully and reminded those students who did not about the pronunciation tips presented in the introductory session. RESULTS Figure 1 presents an overall analysis of results which included all variables. It displays percentage of correct perceptual identifications of schwa words (identified as correct) and full vowel words (identified as incorrect) as indicated by the t‐test analyses carried out. There were not any significant differences in the identification of a spliced schwa vowel as correct between testing stages in any of the tested groups [A (pre‐test: 81.8%; post‐test: 80.6%), B (pre‐test: 74.6%; post‐test: 79.3%) and C (pre‐ test: 74.4; post‐test: 74.6%)] or when analysed together [ABC (pre‐test: 76.9%; post‐test: 78.1%)]. Esther Gómez Lacabex and Francisco Gallardo del Puerto 208 Figure 1. Percent correct identification of all items for schwa words (top) and full vowel words (bottom) for groups A, B and C. For the full vowel words corpus, intra‐group analyses revealed that all three groups underwent significant improvement (A: t = ‐8.45; p<.001; B: t = ‐5.17; p<.001; C: t = ‐3.04; p<.01) in their identification of a spliced full vowel as incorrect in post‐test. Pre‐test means (A: 27.9%; B: 36.5%; C: 33.1%; ABC: 32%) indicated that performance was at low chance levels, possibly revealing a bias towards ‘correct’ identification. Post‐test means (A: 73.5%; B: 60.4%; C: 43.3%; ABC: 58%) indicated that groups A and B could more consistently identify a full vowel as incorrect after training whereas group C remained in chance performance levels. Figure 2 shows results in terms of position of the vowels within the word: pre‐tonic and post‐tonic. In the case of spliced schwa words, results did not reveal intra‐group differences for any of the groups in either pre‐ tonic or post‐tonic position. 20 40 60 80 100 A B C schwa vowel words

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

More Proficient vs. Less Proficient EFL Learners’ Perceptions of Teachers ‘Motivation Raising Strategies

Motivation raising strategies are frequently used in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) classes; nevertheless, learners’ perceptions of such strategies used by language teachers have not sufficiently been explored. Also, there are not enough studies on differences and similarities between more and less proficient EFL learners regarding this issue. To scrutinize this topic, a groups of more (No...

متن کامل

Improving Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners’ Oral Narrative Task Performance in Terms of Accuracy, Fluency and Complexity by Awareness Raising Through Semantic Fields

The effects different awareness-raising techniques might have on language learners’ performance have been studied by many researchers. The present study specifically focused on improving EFL learners’ oral narrative task performance in terms of accuracy, fluency and complexity by awareness raising through semantic fields. The participants in the study included 40 intermediate learners whose ini...

متن کامل

Raising EFL Learners' Pragmatic Awareness of Intercultural Rhetoric in Writing

The purpose of the present study was to explore the effectiveness of raising Iranian learners’ pragmatic awareness of intercultural rhetoric in enhancing their L2 writing ability, complexity, and accuracy. To this end, two 25-member groups of EFL learners who were taking Basic Writing course at Islamic Azad University, Roudehen Branch, were selected based on their performance on a language prof...

متن کامل

Discrimination training of phonemic contrasts enhances phonological processing in mainstream school children.

Auditory perceptual learning has been proposed as effective for remediating impaired language and for enhancing normal language development. We examined the effect of phonemic contrast discrimination training on the discrimination of whole words and on phonological awareness in 8- to 10-year-old mainstream school children. Eleven phonemic contrast continua were synthesised using linear interpol...

متن کامل

The Role of Gender in Reading Comprehension of Short Stories Taught Through Awareness-Raising

The present study examines the role of gender in reading comprehension of short stories taught through awareness-raising among Iranian EFL learners. In so doing, twelve male and eighteen female learners with lower intermediate English proficiency were selected as the research sample. Next, they were randomly divided into two fifteen-member groups (i.e., a control group and an experimental group...

متن کامل

EFL Learners’ Writing Progress through Collocation Awareness-raising Approach: An analytic assessment

There is insufficient rigorous research examining which features of EFL/ESL learners’ writing can be improved through their awareness/knowledge of collocation. This study, therefore, addressed this issue and examined the effect of this awareness on Iranian EFL learners’ writing performance with respect to the specific features of each writing sub-component (i.e., content, organization, vocabula...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2014